1

We have seen the Newton disc, when it is rotated it produces white color. So I want to know whether white exists as a single color or is that a combination of multiple colors?

3 Answers3

6

It depends on your definition of a "color". There is a crayon in my box of coloring pencils that is "white" - you could call that a single color. But if I look at the spectral components of a light source that my eye perceives as white, there could be many different compositions, all of which look "white" to me. It might be "all the colors of the rainbow" (like sunlight), it might be just the right mixture of red, green and blue (like the screen I am looking at while I type this), or it could be another mixture such as is generated by the phosphors of LEDs and fluorescent tubes that attempt to produce "white" light.

See for example this answer and the links contained therein, for more information. In particular, look at Why can colors be mixed? and Colors from a computer vs. colors from visible spectrum of the sun

UPDATE

Following the discussion in the comments, I dug up the spectral characteristics of a couple of different displays from this paper. Clearly there are three peaks ("colors") present in both - while there is a lot of difference between the CCFL and LED displays, there is not a continuous spectrum in either of them.

enter image description here

Floris
  • 118,905
  • Are you using a laser projection screen? Normally LED monitors will have the whole visible spectrum with non-zero spectral density, not just "red", "green" and "blue". E.g. orange and yellow are also there. CCFL-lit monitors also have not only red, green and blue peaks in their spectrum. – Ruslan Jan 12 '16 at 10:38
  • @Ruslan I think he is referring to the fact that the three most common sub pixels that make up a pixel on a modern-day screen are red, green & blue. – James T Jan 12 '16 at 13:00
  • @Ruslan OLED screens have pretty tight spectrum even internally. But backlit LCDs are still filtered so there are no intermediate colors, like orange and yellow, in what actually leaves the monitor. – Agent_L Jan 12 '16 at 15:13
  • @Agent_L my measurements disagree with you. My original comment was based on what I saw via spectrometer. – Ruslan Jan 12 '16 at 15:17
  • @Ruslan You know the drill - pics or it didn't happened : D – Agent_L Jan 12 '16 at 15:23
  • 1
    @Agent_L OK, when I get home. – Ruslan Jan 12 '16 at 15:36
  • @Ruslan - it would be interesting to compare your results with the ones I just added... – Floris Jan 12 '16 at 15:46
  • 2
    @Agent_L So, here they are, with different colors on screen: white, red, green, blue. Monitor is Samsung LS22E391HSX/CI. – Ruslan Jan 12 '16 at 18:59
  • @Ruslan Great, thanks! Oranges and yellow look like parasitic bleed, but indeed the resulting spectrum is continuous (but not even) – Agent_L Jan 13 '16 at 15:23
0

It is simple :

  • When you put all the color from visible spectrum on a surface (so it reflects all wavelengths hitting it), You will get White Color

  • When you extract all the color from visible spectrum on a surface (so it absorbs all wavelengths hitting it), You will get Black Color

Newton demonstrated that white light could be broken up into its composite colors by passing it through a prism, then using a second prism to reassemble them. He also discovered that sunlight is composed of light with wavelengths across the visible spectrum. Concluding that since all colors produce white light then white must be made up of all colors is a common logical error called affirming the consequent, which might be the cause of the misunderstanding.

Source : Wikipedia

See picture below : prism

Iamat8
  • 417
  • 1
  • 4
  • 10
0

Color is just a "projection" of spectrum to what we can perceive. So... almost every color can be reproduced in ifinitely many combinations of different wavelengths! Essentially, your real physical state is a function $j(\lambda)$ that for every wavelength $\lambda$ tells you the intensity of the light. You have an infinite amount of information here, as the curve can be anything.

What you see is the sum of contributions of each wavelength, each with its own efficiency (weight) to the response of three different types of cones (it's a bit more complicated than that, because there is receptor saturation, adaptation, neural post-processing, the rod sensors and so on). Mathematically, you "see" three numbers, each defined as $$\{r,g,b\}=\int_{\text{spectrum}} j(\lambda) \{R(\lambda),G(\lambda),B(\lambda)\}\,d\lambda$$

So as long as these three numbers are the same, the color is the same. In addition to integration over the spectrum, your eye also "integrates" over the response time (it can't keep up, it accumulates everything it receives in some period, some 20-50 "frames per second"). So a spinning disk of colors "fakes" actual addition of multiple spectra by sequencing in time too quickly to separate visually, just as a computer screen puts them too close together to resolve spatially. However, the spectral physics is the same. So...

You know you can make white in many different ways. Sunlight has a black body spectrum (continuous). Then you have all these fluorescent bulbs with somewhat different spectra (with many broad bands), and then you have LEDs with essentially line spectra (single wavelengths). That's true for other colors too. Orange can be a single wavelength (sodium lamp, for instance), or red + yellow wavelength (2 LEDS), or full-red + half-green on a computer screen (again, each of these has its own spectrum) or a continuous stretch of wavelengths between yellow all the way to a bit of green. It matters not, the eye will see them all the same. Well... there's a catch. When you illuminate a sample of one spectral color (which tells which color is reflected in what amount) with a light of some other color... the spectral responses are multiplied ("reflection from the object" times "intensity of incoming light"). So... you can have two light of the same color... and two papers of the same color (but different spectra)... and they will not produce the same result in all combinations. Consider an extreme example where all papers and lights deal with discrete wavelengths: paper A [red + yellow] and paper B [orange]. And light U [orange] and light V [red + 0.5green]. Now illuminate: Paper A under light U looks black: the orange wavelength is not reflected, but absorbed. Paper A under light V looks red: the green component from the light is absorbed and not seen. Paper B under light U is orange. Paper B under light V is again black.

This reason is, why artificial lighting, especially if the spectra are narrow stripes instead of continuous, makes some things look weird and unnatural, and possibly don't display nice on camera (as the entire spectrum is again multiplied by the camera sensor's response function, the type of the camera matters too!). Mostly, it's not as bad as illustrated before, because discrete-wavelength materials are very rare (must be special-made and expensive). But discrete-wavelength lights are more and more common, and color consistency is becoming a problem.

I propose an experiment. Get a yellow light, possibly sodium-based... make two discs... one pure yellow... one half-red half-green. In white light, they may look similar when spinning. Under yellow light, the second should look darker... depending, of course, on the spectral responses of the yellow pigment you use.

orion
  • 6,604
  • It's even worse than what you say because "white" is such a subjective experience. There's a sheet of office paper on my desk, illuminated by an incandescent floodlight. When I look at the sheet of paper, it looks "white". But, when I look at the lamp itself, and compare it to the light coming in through the window, the lamp seems to have a significant orange tint. – Solomon Slow Jan 12 '16 at 15:41